Wednesday, April 18, 2012

An examination of the link between learning styles and course performance OR sorry all of you kinesthetic learners!


I reviewed a paper a few months ago by John Dobson. In his first paper, he examined whether differences exist in the learning styles across “sex, status, and course performance.” I had some criticisms for that paper, which you can find here, and when I came across a second paper written by Dobson, I thought it would be interesting to see what he had to say.
As with the previous paper I reviewed by this author, he is not examining the existence of learning styles. The assumption has been made that they exist. His goal is to link learning styles to class performance. In his introduction, he makes the following statement:
“…when teaching physiology to a diverse group of students, the most thorough and successful strategy is to present information using multiple learning styles.”
Notice the choice of words here. He does not say that the most successful strategy is to present information via multiple sensory modalities or in multiple ways (which there is some research to support). Instead, the wording includes the term “learning styles.” This is a pretty strong statement to make. Is there any evidence provided for this statement? Dobson cites three sources in support of this statement. Let’s examine those.
            The first of the three that I could get my hands on is a paper from 2004 by Kimberly Tanner and Deborah Allen. The paper is entirely theory and provides absolutely no evidence supporting the above quote. There are a few parts of the paper that theoretically address the idea of learning styles and classroom success / teaching pedagogy, but there is no data or evidence provided in the paper. For example, Tanner and Allen state:
“From a biological perspective, the brain is the organ of learning, and as such, a learning style is likely to be a complex, emergent interaction of the neurophysiology of an individual’s brain and the unique developmental process that has shaped it through experience and interaction with the environment. Learning style, thus, is a phenotypic characteristic of an organism like any other. Given the plasticity of the human brain and its propensity to learn and likely change synaptically over time, learning styles should be considered to be flexible, not immutable- an individual’s learning style could be actively adapted, to a certain extent, to different learning environments.”
This is a theory- no evidence is provided that tie learning styles to neurophysiology. In the following paragraph, Tanner and Allen continue with “The study of human learning styles is a well-established field within the discipline of cognitive psychology.” Considering the trouble that I have had with finding evidence within the field of psychology (specifically the areas of cognitive and educational psychology), I must disagree with this statement. They go on to cite Honey and Mumford (1982), Kolb (1984), and Myers Briggs papers (Myers-Briggs, 1980). To my knowledge, these papers are very popular within educational psychology circles, but they are not talked about in most cognitive psychology textbooks nor are they in any psychology book that I could find that was specific to learning and memory (Gluck, Mercado, and Myers, 2008; Malone, 1991).
The other two sources listed are not available for free online. They appear to be largely theoretical, but if you have access to either of these articles and want to send them to me, I would greatly appreciate it (Keller, J. (1987). Development and use of the ARCS model of instructional design. J Instr Develop 10: 2-10. and Miller JA. (1998). Enhancement of achievement and attitudes through individualized learning style presentations of two allied health courses. J Allied Health 27: 150-156.)
Dobson found gender differences in learning style preference (which is different than what was found last time). Both males and females prefer visual learning the most (49% and 46% of them respectively) and kinesthetic learning the least (5% and 4% respectively), but there appear to be differences for the other two learning modalities. Females prefer aural to the read/write modality (27% to 23%) and males prefer read/write to the aural modality (29% to 17%). Although small, these differences were significant. One must question why gender differences were found in this study and not in the other study by this author that I reviewed previously (Dobson, 2010). Going to that paper, they came very close to finding a significant difference between males and females in the study. That said, their distribution of response preferences was dramatically different than in this study (with fewer “visual learners” and more “kinesthetic learners”). Why the substantial variation here? Are we talking about different populations? Both of the studies by Dobson focused on physiology students. If we are trying to classify individuals into various types of learners, yet we cannot reliably characterize the population of learners into subgroups, maybe these subgroups do not exist? The lack of reliability with characterizing the populations examined is worrisome.

Students were asked about which portion of the course they found most helpful. The vast majority of the students selected that the lecture portion of the class was aided by the lecture instructor and by the presentation materials. For the lab portion of the class, the most helpful items were the lab instructor, the lab materials, and the lab manual. The lab activities themselves were listed as most helpful by a very small minority of students. The author claims that this low number is reflective of the very small minority of students that prefer kinesthetic learning (only 4-5% of the students in this class identified this way). I have to wonder just how easily a student could differentiate the individual components of a laboratory into which were most helpful. For example, when I took labs as an undergraduate, the activity was crucial, but the lab instructor circled and helped students with various portions throughout the activity. Which portion is the most helpful? It might be difficult for students to specifically identify whether they were helped more by the activity or by their instructor.

Looking at their data, the “lecture scores for the kinesthetic group were significantly lower than those from the other three groups.” In addition, the “overall course scores from the kinesthetic group were significantly less than those from the other three groups…” The authors discuss that this indicates that kinesthetic learners are largely ignored by current teaching practices in physiology classes in college. If the author is correct, increasing the kinesthetic component of a class should lead to an increase in learning (and thus performance) with the group of “K-learners.” However, instead, all students scored better in the laboratory portion of the class. It could be argued that the kinesthetic group improved more than the other three groups, and while that is true, one must wonder whether the other three groups experienced a ceiling effect of sorts here. They are all averaging/scoring near 90% on the laboratory portion of the class. Maybe this is as high as those group averages can get? Nonetheless, the kinesthetic learners were still scoring below the other groups even when we focus on the much more kinesthetic friendly laboratory portion of the class.

I wish that the author would have provided the F statistics for their ANOVA- They claim to have found a significant interaction between learning style preferences and final scores in the lecture, lab, and overall class, but provide no statistics to back that claim. Overall, this is an interesting paper but it does little to provide us with evidence in favor of learning styles.

References

Dobson, J. (2009). Learning style preferences and course performance in an undergraduate physiology class. Advances in Physiological Education, 33: 308-314.

Dobson, J. (2010). A comparison between learning style preferences and sex, status, and course performance. Advances in Physiological Education, 34: 197-204.

Gluck, M, Mercado, E, Myers, C. Learning and Memory: From Brain to Behavior. 2008, Worth Publishers

Malone, JC. Theories of Learning: A Historical Approach. 1991, Wadsworth.

Tanner, K, Allen, D. (2004). Approaches to biology teaching and learning: learning styles and the problem of instructional selection-engaging all students in science courses. Cell Biol Educ 31: 153-157.


2 comments:

  1. This comment has been removed by a blog administrator.

    ReplyDelete
  2. Though I am no neurophysiologist, my experience and instinct (emotional intelligence) lead me to concur with John Dobson, whose writings Mr. Smith is examining, and also to disagree with Dobson. Concurring: I see that primary learning styles can and do change with age and circumstances. Taking myself as an example, as a young adult the inventories that I took revealed that I was primarily a visual learner; now that I'm nearly 60, the latest VAK inventory strongly suggested that I operate best in the kinesthetic learning style.
    Disagreeing: Dobson's study indicated that "The lab activities themselves were listed as most helpful by a very small minority of students...that this low number is reflective of the very small minority of students that prefer kinesthetic learning." Well, there is more touchy-feely stuff in the lab that just the "lab activities." There are the lab manual, the lab environment, and the lab instructors. All of these speak well to the K-learner.
    I'm wondering if those learning styles instruments that I participated in back in the 1980's were as good as the modern ones. Was I really a V-learner back then? Have I transitioned to a K-learner, or have I always preferred the kinesthetic learning style. I've not yet read anything (and I'm certainly not well- nor widely-read on the subject) that convinces me of anything except that learning styles, or learning paths, do truly exist.

    ReplyDelete